<NOTE>
An Old Female Bonobo Carried a Dead Red-Tailed Monkey for over a Month

Kazuya Toda1, Nahoko Tokuyama2, & Takeshi Furuichi1
1 Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Japan
2 The Graduate University for Advanced Studies, Japan


INTRODUCTION

Carrying infants is one of the most important maternal tasks determining the survival of dependent infants in non-human primates (Nicolson 1987; Ross 2001). Even after the death of their infants, mothers tend to carry the carcasses for several days, and even up to several months (Hosaka et al. 2000; Sugiyama et al. 2009; Tokuyama et al. 2017). Such prolonged carrying can be considered as “maternal persistency” (Sugiyama et al. 2009; Biro et al. 2010).

Excluding mothers that carry their infants, non-human primates rarely carry objects for a long period, (Ross 2001; Carvalho et al. 2012). However, at Wamba in Democratic Republic of the Congo, we observed that an old female bonobo (Pan paniscus) carried the carcass of a red-tailed monkey (Cercopithecus ascanius) for over a month. We here report occasional observations of her behaviors and conditions of the monkey carcass, and discuss why she had been persistent with the dead body of another primate.


SITE AND SUBJECT

Observations were made in 2016 at Wamba, Luo Scientific Reserve, Democratic Republic of Congo, which is a long-term field site for the study of wild bonobos (Kano 1992). There are two adjacent groups of bonobos, the eastern group (PE) and the western group (PW), of which all members were fully habituated and identified. We usually followed the PE group from one night’s bed sites (around 06:00 h) to the next night’s bed sites (around 17:00 h), and occasionally observed the PW group especially during inter-group encounters. The adult female reported in the present study, Chio (estimated to be 45–50 years old), had been identified in the PW group together with her daughter (Chacha: estimated to be 7–8 years old) in September 2012. Since then, Chio had not been observed to have an infant.


OBSERVATIONS

On August 18, we found that Chio was carrying the dead body of a red-tailed monkey at 13:47 h. The monkey carcass seemed to be an adult based on size, which was as large as infant bonobos at one or two years of age. It did not have any visible external injury and retained its head, all limbs, tail, and fur. It was suspected to have died several days before the observation because it had already decayed and emitted a decomposing odor to the observers. When Chio rested with the carcass on a tree at 14:00 h, two adult males approached her to watch it but did not try to take it. At 14:34 h, Chio hung it on her neck like a muffler and moved away with it.

On August 25, we found that Chio was with the monkey carcass at 13:40 h, but we could observe her only once on that day.

On August 29, we found that Chio walked on the ground with the carcass hanging on her neck at 12:46 h. The decay of the carcass was proceeding. Little fur remained and it was dried up from the head to the tail, including all the limbs. Chio grasped the carcass in her hand and engaged in genito-genital rubbing with another adult female at 12:48 h, then she moved away hanging it on her neck.

On August 31, we found Chio resting on a tree with the carcass hung around her neck at 06:43 h. One infant approached Chio apparently to play with the carcass at 07:13 h and Chio moved the carcass from her neck onto her abdominal region to prevent the infant from touching the carcass. At 07:21 h after the infant left, Chio lifted the head of the carcass to eye level and gazed at it for about five minutes. She groomed it for two minutes at 07:36 h, occasionally flapping flies gathering around it, and moved away with it at 07:40 h.

On September 17, we found that Chio had the mummified monkey at 1649 h, but we only caught a glimpse of her once before members made night beds.

On September 21, we found Chio was still in possession of the carcass at 07:14 h. The monkey carcass was fully mummified. When she engaged in genito-genital rubbing with another adult female and a juvenile female at 07:34 h, she grasped the mummy in her hand. Then, she rested and held the mummy in her abdominal region at 07:40 h (Figure 1, also see Video 1 available online at http://mahale.main.jp/PAN/24_2/24(2)_04.html). One infant approached her and watched the mummy at 07:56 h. She suddenly grabbed the arm of the infant, and the infant ran away screaming. Chio groomed the mummy with her mouth at 08:49 h, and moved away with it hanging on her neck.



Figure 1. Chio resting on a tree with a mummified red-tailed monkey.



Video 1

On September 29 and 30, we found that Chio still walked with the mummy hanging around her neck at 11:23h and13:40h. The mummy was intact until our last observation.

On October 25, we found Chio within members of the PW group at 10:18 h, but she was no longer carrying the monkey carcass.


DISCUSSION

Chio had carried the dead red-tailed monkey for at least 43 days without a motivation of preying or playing, although we did not observe how she got and released it. She was observed to groom the hair of the monkey carcass, gaze at its body, and defend it from curious juveniles. She seemed to handle the monkey carcass carefully and protectively even during it had been decaying. Her behaviors towards the decaying monkey carcass could be considered similar to the behaviors of mothers carrying their dead infants with great care (Kano 1992; Hosaka et al. 2000; Sugiyama et al. 2009; Biro et al. 2010; Tokuyama et al. 2017).

Few observations of non-human primates carrying bodies of other animal species for a long time have been reported until date. In chimpanzees, however, a barren female carried a limb of dead dried red colobus (Procolobus rufomitratus) for seven hours at Mahale (Hosaka et al. 2000), and an adolescent female carried a dead hyrax (Dendrohyrax dorsalis) for fifteen hours, slept with it in her nest, and groomed it at Bossou (Hirata et al. 2001). These observations might indicate that their imagination could make an unusual perception and reaction towards other animal species.

Allomothering, defined as temporary carrying of non-kin infants is performed mostly by non-parous young females, which has been suggested to be a process of acquiring maternal skills in non-human primates (McKenna 1979). However, it is occasionally conducted by adult parous females without their own infants (Nozaki 2009; Tokuyama 2015). Chio’s case fits the latter well, because she had not cared her own infants for a long time. The unusual carrying might have been a coincidence induced by the maternal drive for infant caring.


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We sincerely thank the Ministry of Scientific Research of the Democratic Republic of Congo for granting permission to conduct research on bonobos and the Research Center on Ecology and Forestry (CREF) in the Democratic Republic of Congo for supporting our research in the Luo Scientific Reserve. We sincerely thank Dr. Sakamaki for his great contribution to continuous observations and camp management at Wamba and the local assistants for their helps of our observations. Field research of KT was financially supported by Leading Program of Primatology and Wildlife Science (PWS) of Kyoto University and Grant-in-Aid for JSPS Fellows (17J01336), Japan.


REFERENCES

Biro D, Humle T, Koops K et al. 2010. Chimpanzee mothers at Bossou, Guinea carry the mummified remains of their dead infants. Curr Biol 20:R351–R352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.031

Carvalho S, Biro D, Cunha E, Matsuzawa T 2012. Chimpanzee carrying behavior and the origins of human bipedality. Curr Biol 22:180–181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.052

Hirata S, Yamakoshi G, Fujita S, Ohashi G, Matsuzawa T 2001. Capturing and toying with hyraxes (Dendrohyrax dorsalis) by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) at Bossou, Guinea. Am J Primatol 53:93–97. https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-2345(200102)53:2<93::AID-AJP5>3.0.CO;2-X

Hosaka K, Matsumoto-Oda A, Huffman MA, Kawanaka K 2000. Reactions to dead bodies of conspecifics by wild chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Primate Res 16:1–15, in Japanese with an English summary. https://doi.org/10.2354/psj.16.1

Kano T 1992. The Last Ape: Pygmy Chimpanzee Behavior and Ecology. Stanford University Press, Stanford, CA.

McKenna JJ 1979. The evolution of allomothering behavior among colobine monkeys: Function and opportunism in evolution. Am Anthropol 81:818–840. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1979.81.4.02a00040

Nicolson NA 1987. Infants, mothers and others females. In: Primates Societies. Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangaham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds), The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 330–342.

Nozaki M 2009. Grandmothers care for orphans in a provisioned troop of Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Primates 50:85–88. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-008-0118-x

Ross C 2001. Park or ride?: Evolution of infant carrying in primates. Int J Primatol 22:749–771. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1012065332758

Sugiyama Y, Kurita T, Matsui T, Kimoto S, Shimomura T 2009. Carrying of dead infants by Japanese macaque (Macaca fuscata) mothers. Anthropol Sci 117:113–119. https://doi.org/10.1537/ase.080919

Tokuyama N 2015. A case of infant carrying against the mother’s will by an old adult female bonobo at Wamba, Democratic Republic of Congo. Pan Afr News 22:15–17. https://doi.org/2433/203112

Tokuyama N, Moore DL, Graham KE, Lokasola A, Furuichi T 2017. Cases of maternal cannibalism in wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) from two different field sites, Wamba and Kokolopori, Democratic Republic of the Congo. Primates 58:7–12. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-016-0582-7



Back to Contents