<NOTE>
Preliminary Report on Hand-Clasp Grooming in Sanctuary-Released Chimpanzees, Haut Niger National Park, Guinea


Tatyana Humle1, Christelle Colin2, & Estelle Raballand3
1Wildlife Research Center, University of Kyoto, Japan
2Projet Primates France, France and Chimpanzee Conservation Center, Faranah, Republic of Guinea
3Project Primate Inc., USA and Chimpanzee Conservation Center, Faranah, Republic of Guinea


INTRODUCTION

Grooming hand-clasp (GHC) was the first social custom labeled as such in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)1,2. This behavior is characterized by variations in form (sometimes also referred to as conventions3 or types2) and in frequency of occurrence (ranging from absent to customary) whether between or within wild chimpanzee communities2 or captive colonies4,5. GHC presents three basic distinct forms: palm to palm1, non-palm to palm2 and wrist to wrist6. This behavior involves two individuals sitting face to face, typically extending their arms overhead (some flexed variations have also been described2) thus clasping or resting their hands or arms, and grooming each other with the other hand1,2.

Among long-term study sites, with the exception of Gombe, Tanzania and Budongo, Uganda, GHC has been noted at all East African2,7. While wrist to wrist GHC is habitually observed at Fongoli, Senegal, the other two forms have more rarely been witnessed at this site (Pruetz pers. com.). At Bossou in Guinea, GHC has only been recorded once (Yamakoshi pers. obs.), and, at Taï in Côte d'Ivoire, the behavior emerged spontaneously and disappeared within a year in two communities (Boesch pers. comm.). GHC has more recently been reported at other sites with semi-habituated individuals, i.e. chimpanzees at Semliki, Uganda (see report in this issue) and bonobos (Pan paniscus) in Lui Kotal in the Democratic Republic of Congo8. We report here on the first record of GHC in sanctuary-released chimpanzees (P. t. verus).

STUDY SITES AND SUBJECTS

The Haut Niger National Park (HNNP) is located in the center of the Republic of Guinea, West Africa. This park was created in 1997 and extends over c. 10,000 km square9,10 (see Fig. 1). It is the largest protected area in the country and is one of the last remaining important formations of dry forest-savanna mosaics in West Africa. This park is also a priority site for the conservation of the Western subspecies of chimpanzee (P. t. verus)11, with a viable population estimated at 500 individuals12,13,14.

Fig. 1. Map of the location of the Haut Niger National Park in Guinea, West Africa, highlighting the Mafou core area of the park and the relative locations of the sanctuary (Somoria) and the release holding facility (Enclosure) (adapted from Brugiere et al. (2005)10).

The Chimpanzee Conservation Center (CCC), the only chimpanzee sanctuary in Guinea, was created in 1997. This sanctuary is located in Somoria within the Mafou core area of the HNNP (see Fig. 1). Twelve chimpanzees between 8-20 years old were released by the CCC in June 2008 in the HNNP, 30 km from the sanctuary15 (see Table 1). The CCC's chimpanzee release project is the first in West Africa and the second of its kind in Africa after the HELP-Congo project in the Democratic Republic of Congo. This preliminary report concerns all 12 recently released chimpanzees from the CCC sanctuary. Data presented here are based on ad libitum observations of GHC by the authors, including a volunteer veterinarian, Carole Geernaert.

OBSERVATIONS

Four instances of GHC have so far been noted. The first instance was witnessed in the holding facility at the release site prior to the release, while the other three were observed post release. Only 3 adult males performed this social custom. One of these adult males, Albert, was involved in all cases observed.

Three of the 4 events presented the palm to palm form of GHC (see Fig. 2). However the first instance, which was recorded in the chimpanzees' holding facility prior release, did not consistently involve straight overhead extension of the arms, as the chimpanzees eventually sat face to face, clasping hands and bracing their elbows on their knees, whilst grooming one another's forearm (see Fig. 2). The wrist to wrist form (which then graded into a more relaxed form during the course of the same bout-see Fig. 3b) was observed only once (see Fig. 3a and Fig 3b). The latter event involved Albert and Rappa. The former supported the wrist of his subordinate grooming partner as illustrated in Figure 3. In effect, the non-palm to palm form of GHC was never observed.

Fig. 2. Palm to palm form of grooming hand-clasp among adults male chimpanzees from the Chimpanzee Conservation Center. Here the arm has been lowered, but the hand-clasping is still very clear. At the same time, a third male chimpanzee is sitting behind; grooming one of the males of the GHC dyad (© Carole Geernaert)

Fig. 3. GHC among released male chimpanzees from the Chimpanzee Conservation Center: (a) sideways wrist to wrist form at the start of the bout with Albert facing the camera and obstructed from view by Rappa (© Chris Colin) and (b) later during the grooming bout, when Rappa took a rest from grooming, Albert continued to groom Rappa with his fingers/palm wrapped around Rappa's forearm and Rappa's left hand/wrist draped loosely over Albert's forearm. Contrary to McGrew et al.6's findings from Mahale, Tanzania, in this case it was the dominant male who was supporting the arm of the subordinate male (© Tatyana Humle).

Although details on specific bout duration of the clasping posture were not precisely recorded at the time of observation, bouts lasted for several minutes, two exceeded 10 minutes in duration. Three of the 4 instances of GHC followed a context of reunion between the males involved (in the presence of adult females in only one case) after several months of separation. We unfortunately have no record or recollection of the context that preceded the first event, but we know it took place in the absence of adult females.Although details on specific bout duration of the clasping posture were not precisely recorded at the time of observation, bouts lasted for several minutes, two exceeded 10 minutes in duration. Three of the 4 instances of GHC followed a context of reunion between the males involved (in the presence of adult females in only one case) after several months of separation. We unfortunately have no record or recollection of the context that preceded the first event, but we know it took place in the absence of adult females.it took place in the absence of adult females.

DISCUSSION

This brief account describes GHC among recently released chimpanzees at the Chimpanzee Conservation Center in Guinea. It is clear that other instances of GHC may have gone unnoticed and that therefore the emergence was never witnessed. However, CCC staff members and researchers now all agree that this behavior occurs rarely, although all chimpanzees regularly engage in mutual social grooming, including adult females. Albert was involved in all instances of GHC witnessed so far. Both Albert and Rappa were rescued as infants, and although Robert was rescued at the age of 7, he had spent many years alone in captivity and had most likely been captured as an infant as well. It is, therefore, unlikely that any of these three males had prior knowledge of and experience with this behavior. One of them may have acquired it by interacting with another individual at the sanctuary or alternatively one of these three males many have innovated this behavior.

Continued data collection on frequency, propagation, i.e. individuals involved and initiating GHC, context prior to grooming, and duration of bouts, as well as form adopted, is underway. But preliminary data presented here suggest so far that GHC in this group may be 1) male-biased (however, so far adult females have had few opportunities to witness this behavior), 2) longer in its bout durations than reported elsewhere and 3) context-specific ensuing reunion events post lengthy periods of separation, characteristics which differ from other published reports concerning this social custom whether in captivity or in the wild.

Table. 1. List of the 12 released chimpanzees with details of their age and sex and observed individual frequency of GHC.

Indeed, based on 12 years of data at Yerkes, GHC bout mean duration was 71.4 sec. and presented no sex-bias4. Among Kanywara chimpanzees in the forest of Kibale, Uganda, GHC bouts last on average 47 sec. (Wrangham pers. comm.), while at Mahale bouts are significantly shorter lasting on average only 37 sec. in the M-group and 22 sec. in the K-group2. At both sites, both adult female and male individuals engaged in this behavior. In addition, no consistent specific context preceding GHC has ever been noted in the wild or in captivity, although de Waal16 described an atypical succession of GHC involving an adolescent male and multiple partners after he was reintroduced into the Yerkes colony after a lengthy absence. This incident was interpreted as reflecting a sense of "belonging" to the group. Preliminary data presented here may corroborate such a hypothesis6,16 at least among males, but it is still too early to conclude anything on the potential symbolic significance of this social custom in the CCC's release group.

Finally, one of the forms reported here may differ from variations observed elsewhere (see Fig. 2) (variations of GHC have been described in detail17). We hope that future data and observations of GHC among this group of released individuals may shed further light on its function, its propagation (as it relates to rank6, experience and affiliation4) and its potential symbolic and cultural significance.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We like to thank the local and expatriate staff at the Center for the Conservation of Chimpanzees (CCC) for their invaluable help and assistance with the chimpanzee release and with care and rehabilitation of chimpanzees held at the sanctuary. We are also grateful to Dr. Mamby Keita, national director of the Direction Nationale de la Recherche Scientifique et Technologique and Mr. Aboubacar Oulare, national director of the Direction de la Diversité Biologique et des Aires Protégées, for their continued support of the CCC's activities in the Haut Niger National Park. in the Republic of Guinea. We would also like to thank and acknowledge the Arcus Foundation and the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Services for their financial support of the release program.

REFERENCES

  1. McGrew WC, Tutin CEG 1978. Evidence for a social custom in wild chimpanzees? Man 13: 234-251.
  2. Nakamura M 2002. Grooming hand-clasp in Mahale M group chimpanzees: implications for culture in social behaviors. In: Behavioural Variation in Chimpanzees and Bonobos, Boesch C, Hohmann, G, Marchant LF eds., Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 71-83.
  3. Whiten A 2005. The second inheritance system of chimpanzees and humans. Nature 437: 52-5.
  4. Bonnie KE, de Waal FBM 2006. Affiliation promotes the transmission of a social custom: handclasp grooming among captive chimpanzees. Primates 47: 27-34.
  5. de Waal FBM, Seres M 1997. Propagation of handclasp grooming among captive chimpanzees. Am J Primatol 43: 339-346.
  6. McGrew WC, Marchant LF, Scott SE, Tutin CEG 2001. Inter-group differences in a social custom of wild chimpanzees: the grooming hand-clasp of the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Curr Anthropol 42: 148-153.
  7. McGrew WC 2004. The Cultured Chimpanzee. Cambridge University Press, New York.
  8. Fruth BI, Hohmann G, Beuerlein MM, McGrew WC 2006. Grooming Hand Clasp by Bonobos of Lui Kotal, Democratic Republic of Congo. Pan Africa News 13: 6-8.
  9. Ziegler S, Nikolaus G, Hutterer R 2002. High mammalian diversity in the newly established National Park of Upper Niger, Republic of Guinea. Oryx 36: 73-80.Ziegler S, Nikolaus G, Hutterer R 2002. High mammalian diversity in the newly established National Park of Upper Niger, Republic of Guinea. Oryx 36: 73-80.
  10. Brugiere D, Dia M, Diakite S, Gbansara M, Mamy M, Saliou B, Magassouba B 2005. Large- and medium-sized ungulates in the Haut Niger National Park, Republic of Guinea: population changes 1997-2002. Oryx 39: 50-55.
  11. Kormos R, Boesch C, Bakarr MI, Butynski TM (eds) 2003. West African Chimpanzees: Status Survey and Conservation Action Plan. UCN/SSC Primate Specialist Group: IUCN, Gland, Switzerland and Cambridge, UK.
  12. Fleury-Brugiere M-C, Brugiere D 2002. Estimation de la population et analyse du comportement nidificateur des chimpanzés dans la zone intégralement protégée Mafou du Parc National du Haut-Niger. Report on the Parc National du Haut-Niger/AGIR project, Faranah.
  13. Brugiere D in press. Key population of the endangered western chimpanzee Pan troglodytes verus in the Haut Niger National Park, Republic of Guinea. Int J Primatol.
  14. Humle T, Deniau C, Lapeyre V, Houpline C, Colin C, Raballand E, in review. Diurnal primates and large- and medium-sized ungulates in the Haut Niger National Park, Republic of Guinea: Update on status, abundance and threats.
  15. Raballand E 2009. Annual Report 2009. Project Primate Inc., Chimpanzee Conservation Center, Faranah, Republic of Guinea.
  16. de Waal FBM 2001. The Ape and the Sushi Master. New York, Basic Books.
  17. Nakamura M, Uehara S 2004. Proximate factors of different types of grooming hand-clasp in Mahale chimpanzees: Implications for chimpanzee social customs. Curr Anthropol 45: 108-114.




Back to Contents