<NOTE>
Laterality in Termite-Fishing by Fongoli Chimpanzees: Preliminary Report


Paco Bertolani1, Clarissa Scholes1, Jill D. Pruetz1, and William C. McGrew2
1Leverhulme Centre for Human Evolutionary Studies, Dpt. of Biological Anthropology, University of Cambridge, UK
2Dpt. of Anthropology, Iowa State University, USA


INTRODUCTION

Many studies in both free-ranging and captive apes have shown that some forms of laterality of hand function occur in non-human primates1. However, true handedness (sensu McGrew and Marchant2), when most individuals show a skew in hand preference in the same direction across different tasks, seems to be restricted to humans. Other hominoids appear unlateralized in simpler tasks, such as reaching, picking up objects, and grooming3, but they show hand preference for more complex tasks, such as tool-using2, 4, 5 or elaborate food processing6, 7.

Laterality in termite-fishing8 has been studied only at Gombe, and the two published data-sets are congruent. McGrew and Marchant2, 9 reported that most (27 of 36) chimpanzees showed an individualized hand preference for right or left, as did Lonsdorf and Hopkins10 (16 of 17) for termite-fishing in the same community. No other data have been published for chimpanzee communities elsewhere. This study asks if termite-fishing by Fongoli chimpanzees is lateralized, shows hand preference (individuals are lateralized, but with no populational preference for either hand), or task specialization (all or most individuals use the same hand).

METHODS

Fongoli, in southeastern Senegal, is a mosaic savanna, and the only hot, dry and open study site with habituated chimpanzees (see Pruetz, 200611, for a description of the habitat and the diet of Fongoli chimpanzees). The only published study of termite-fishing at Fongoli is based on indirect data12 (Bogart and Pruetz, in prep.).

PB collected these data in April and May 2005, after habituation of the first males of the Fongoli community. The chimpanzees were observed for 543 hours over 50 days, when termites were a major food source. PB collected data using scan sampling with a focal subject target on the subject's activity and on the other individuals in sight, by instantaneous recording every 5 minutesPB collected these data in April and May 2005, after habituation of the first males of the Fongoli community. The chimpanzees were observed for 543 hours over 50 days, when termites were a major food source. PB collected data using scan sampling with a focal subject target on the subject's activity and on the other individuals in sight, by instantaneous recording every 5 minutesPB collected these data in April and May 2005, after habituation of the first males of the Fongoli community. The chimpanzees were observed for 543 hours over 50 days, when termites were a major food source. PB collected data using scan sampling with a focal subject target on the subject's activity and on the other individuals in sight, by instantaneous recording every 5 minutes13.

A session of termite-fishing was a continuous period during which the individual ate termites. Sessions are separated by other activities (e.g. rest, groom, etc). During a session, change of posture or position and change of tool occurred: thus a session contained several bouts (sensu McGrew and Marchant2). The data-points taken in each session by instantaneous sampling cannot be considered independent observations, so statistical analysis was done only on individual sessions, which do not equal events2. Laterality was tested with the binomial test for each individual who attained at least 6 sessions. All tests are two-tailed, with alpha set at 0.05 (A further 10 chimpanzees showed termite-fishing but did not reach this criterion, which is the minimum number necessary to show a statistic significance).

PB recorded which hand inserted and extracted the tool into and from the hole in the termite mound for each insertion event.

RESULTS

Table 1 shows the data for 47 sessions of termite-fishing shown by 5 males. Each session averaged 9.1 data-points (range per individual: 7.3-10.6) and lasted an average of 45 minutes. Two individuals, BN and KL, were exclusively lateralized to the left and right respectively; YO was significantly right lateralized, and MM and SI were right-biased and left-biased respectively, but not significantly so. Although the sample is very small, no skew emerges in the direction of hand preference among these subjects. The results for instantaneous samples reflect those for sessions.


Table 1. Frequency of lateralized (L vs. R) termite-fishing by sessions and instantaneous samples.



DISCUSSION

Termite-fishing at Fongoli appears to be an individually lateralized task, in which some of the individuals specialize in using one hand, and some use the other, but none is ambilateral. These results agree with studies of Gombe chimpanzees3, 9, 10, despite the sites being separated by thousands of kilometers on opposite sides of Africa. Extending data collection to the rest of the Fongoli community is needed to validate these preliminary findings.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank the Republic of Senegal and Department of Eaux et Forets for permission to work in Senegal; D. Kante and M. Camara for assistance in the field. J.P. was supported by grants from the National Geographic Society, Center for the Study of Violence at Iowa State University (ISU), ISU Foreign Travel Grant, ISU Faculty Professional Development Grant, and the American Society of Primatologists Conservation Grant.

REFERENCES

  1. McGrew WC, Marchant LF 1997. On the other hand: Current issues in and meta-analysis of the behavioral laterality of hand function in nonhuman primates. Yearb Phys Anthropol 40: 201–232.
  2. McGrew WC, Marchant LF 1996. On which side of the apes? Ethological study of laterality of hand use. In: Great Ape Societies, McGrew WC, Marchant LF, Nishida T (eds), Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 255–272.
  3. Marchant LF, McGrew WC 1996. Laterality of limb function in wild chimpanzees of Gombe National Park: Comprehensive study of spontaneous activities. J Hum Evol 30: 427–443.
  4. Boesch C 1991. Handedness in wild chimpanzees. Int J Primatol 12: 541–558.
  5. Sugiyama Y, Fushimi T, Sakura O, Matsuzawa T 1993. Hand preference and tool use in wild chimpanzees. Primates 34: 151–159.
  6. Byrne RW, Byrne JME 1993. Complex leaf-gathering skills of mountain gorillas (Gorilla g. beringei): Variability and standardization. Am J Primatol 31: 241–261.
  7. Corp N, Byrne RW 2002. Leaf processing by wild chimpanzees: Physically defended leaves reveal complex manual skills. Ethology 108: 673–696.
  8. Goodall J 1963. Feeding behaviour of wild chimpanzees. A preliminary report. Symp Zool Soc Lond 10: 39–47.
  9. McGrew WC, Marchant LF 1992. Chimpanzees, tools, and termites: Hand preference or handedness? Curr Anthropol 33: 114–119.
  10. Lonsdorf EV, Hopkins WD 2005. Wild chimpanzees show population-level handedness for tool use. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102: 12634–12638.
  11. Pruetz JD 2006. Feeding ecology of savanna chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) at Fongoli, Senegal. In: Feeding Ecology in Apes and Other Primates: Ecological, Physical and Social Aspects, Hohmann G, Robbins MM, Boesch C (eds), New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 161–182.
  12. McGrew WC, Pruetz JD, Fulton SJ 2005. Chimpanzees use tools to harvest social insects at Fongoli, Senegal. Folia Primatol 76: 222–226.
  13. Altmann J 1974. Observational study of behaviour: sampling methods. Behaviour 49: 227–265.


Back to Contents